1132331 research-article2022 JHPXXX10.1177/00221678221132331Journal of Humanistic PsychologyTweed et al. Article Humanistic Optimal Functioning Predicts Low Youth Violence Journal of Humanistic Psychology 1­–23 © The Author(s) 2022 Article reuse guidelines: sagepub.com/journals-permissions https://doi.org/10.1177/00221678221132331 DOI: 10.1177/00221678221132331 journals.sagepub.com/home/jhp Roger G. Tweed1 , Gira Bhatt1, Stephen Dooley2, Jodi L. Viljoen3, Kevin S. Douglas3, and Nathalie Gagnon1 Abstract This study assessed whether indicators of humanistic optimal functioning were predictive of lower levels of violence among youth across a 6-month period. Youth (N = 346) aged 12 to 14 years completed measures of authenticity and of positive regard for others (generalized trust, forgiveness, and gratitude). Approximately 6 months later, the youth reported violence, criminal offenses, and indicators of potential violence, and for some (n = 266), a teacher provided ratings of aggression. Authentic living, some elements of generalized trust, forgiveness, and gratitude predicted lower levels on indicators of aggression or violence or readiness for violence 6 months later. The relation between humanistic predictors and violence-related outcomes was larger for youth at elevated risk for violence. Unexpectedly, a subtype of authenticity, “resisting external influence,” predicted higher violence, but other outcomes were in the expected direction. Thus, a humanistic lens may have value in examinations of societal violence. Keywords humanistic, violence, aggression, positive regard, at risk, authenticity, gratitude 1 Kwantlen Polytechnic University, Surrey, BC, Canada Simon Fraser University, Surrey, BC, Canada 3 Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada 2 Corresponding Author: Roger G. Tweed, Department of Psychology, Kwantlen Polytechnic University, 12666 – 72nd Avenue, Surrey, BC V3W 2M8, Canada. Email: roger.tweed@kpu.ca 2 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) This longitudinal study assessed whether indicators of humanistic optimal functioning were predictive of lower levels of violence-related outcomes among youth across a 6-month period. To humanistic psychologists, it may seem self-evident that humanistic ideals, such as authenticity, positive regard for others, and other indicators of optimal development will reduce violence because psychological health will negate the need for violence. Consistent with this view, humanistic approaches to violence have been proposed (e.g., Atieno Fisher, 2003). Although the impact of humanistic optimal functioning on violence may seem self-evident for some humanistic psychologists, the ideas seldom receive mention from violence researchers (Coupland & Olver, 2020). If the relevance of humanistic psychology was broadly evident to people not identifying as humanistic psychologists, then one would expect constructs, such as authenticity and positive regard, to be central in both violence theory and assessment. The relative dearth of attention is understandable, given that, to outsiders, attention to violence may appear contrary to humanistic psychology’s essence. Maslow (1954), for example, is often noted for his famous claim that research focused on stunted, immature, and unhealthy participants will produce a disabled psychology. However, humanistic psychologists may know that in the book containing that famous quote, Maslow also wrote extensively about psychotherapy, indicating his great interest in and concern for pathology. Similarly, Carl Rogers (2012) engaged extensively with therapeutic work, again indicating attention to pathology. He also addressed racism, overpopulation, over-policing, and unjust war (Rogers, 1972), and others have followed this humanistic tradition of attention to pathology (Franco et al., 2020; Morrill, 2021). Thus, there is value in generating data explicitly testing the link between humanistic ideals and violence. This study assesses predictive power across 6 months. Violence researchers may be more open to humanistic concepts than they were in the past, as demonstrated by recent research on protective factors (Coupland & Olver, 2020; de Vogel et al., 2011; Muir et al., 2020). However, one must be careful when reading literature on protective factors because “protective,” as used within these sources, does not necessarily mean factors that cause reduced violence, but instead means factors predicting lower levels of violence. Protective factors include intelligence, positive attitude toward authority, leisure activities, positive home, school, and community environments, prosocial involvement, and social support (de Vogel et al., 2011; Muir et al., 2020; Zych et al., 2019). Clearly, positive environments are relevant to humanistic psychology, but given the humanistic focus on the optimal Tweed et al. 3 functioning within individuals, further attention to optimal functioning and protection may also deserve attention. Humanistic psychology includes attention to many variables, but only a few variables that seemed particularly relevant to violence were examined in this assessment. Authenticity has long been a central construct for humanistic psychology (Sohmer, 2020). Wood et al. (2008) defined authenticity as “to know yourself and act accordingly.” This definition maps closely onto Rogers’ (1957) concept of congruence. Prior to conducting this study, we gathered a community coalition to provide feedback. They expressed concern about gang violence, so we included questions about gangs. They also suggested measuring authenticity. The theoretical link between authenticity and violence might not seem obvious; however, Rogers (1957) believed that incongruent people (i.e., people not acting congruently with their true self) are prone to anxiety and disorganization. If he was correct, then inauthenticity could predict lashing out in frustration from a disorganized personal state rather than living in a mindful state (Tohme & Joseph, 2020). In contrast, authenticity may promote tolerance of others who disagree (Sartre, 1975). In addition, authenticity involves a willingness to stand for one’s own beliefs (Wood et al., 2008), which may help youth resist peer pressure supporting violence. Another central construct in humanistic psychology is positive regard for others. Carl Rogers (1980) believed that the mature person would emanate positive regard for others, and that it would positively affect others. He described potatoes that grew green stalks when planted in nutritious soil but when stored in the dark basement of his childhood home, they grew twisted white stalks. He argued that his clients were the same. If he, as an optimally functioning human, provided conditions of growth, including positive regard, the clients’ growth tendency could be trusted. Readiness to have positive regard for others underlies much of humanistic psychology. Positive regard could possibly manifest as generalized trust, a belief that most people are trustworthy. Regions with higher belief that people are trustworthy have far lower rates of violence (Elgar & Aitken, 2011), possibly because low trust produces readiness to protect oneself against the anticipated malicious acts of others and, possibly, even readiness to use violence (Buber, 1957). In contrast, youth who have more positive views of human nature seem more likely to support world peace (Adams, 1989). Generalized trust does not represent all of positive regard for others, so it is important to assess other indicators of positive regard (Tweed et al., 2021). For example, forgiveness involves a willingness to let go of resentment toward others who have wronged the self and, in contrast, hold a positive or at least not negative view of them. This too may protect against violence (García-Vázquez et al., 2020; Tweed et al., 2011), possibly because 4 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) forgiveness displaces vengeful attitudes (Berry et al., 2005) that drive much violence (Kennedy, 2011). Another indicator of positive regard is gratitude, a willingness to look in the past and recognize that another has given something of value to the self. It is a willingness to see positive aspects in another’s contribution to the self. Gratitude may have an equally important, but less obvious relation to violence (García-Vázquez et al., 2020; Tweed et al., 2011). Gratitude promotes prosocial intentions (Tsang, 2006) and predicts lower levels of antisocial behavior (Bono et al., 2019). Violent intentions may tend to dissipate if one experiences gratitude. Indicators of optimal functioning may not elicit much interest from violence researchers unless they provide predictive power beyond that provided by widely used indicators of risk, so we also assessed risk in this study. Prior analyses indicated relevant predictive risk factors of violence, including school failure, impulsivity, and peer delinquency (Otto & Douglas, 2010). The hypotheses for this study were as follows: Hypothesis 1 (H1): Humanistic optimal functioning, as assessed with measures of authenticity and positive regard for others (generalized trust, forgiveness, and gratitude), predicts lower levels of violence and potential for violence 6 months later (partial correlations controlling for gender and age). Hypothesis 2 (H2): Humanistic optimal functioning contributes longitudinally predictive power beyond that provided by the risk indicators [impulsivity, peer delinquency, and school failure] (tested with hierarchical linear regressions entering demographics, then risk factors, and then optimal functioning). Hypothesis 3 (H3): The effect is comparable at varying levels of risk. In other words, in longitudinal prediction of violence-related outcomes, the interaction between optimal functioning and risk does not approach significance (i.e., p > .20). Method Participants The 346 participants were aged between 12 and 14 years (M = 13.1, SD = .40) and included 157 males and 189 females. Participants were recruited from Grades 8 and 9 of public school classes in Western Canada. On average, 58% of the students received the required active parental consent and participated in the questionnaire. The most reported ethnicities were South Asian Tweed et al. 5 (38% of the sample), European (13%, although a further 4% reported their ethnicity as Canadian, and, based on the local demographics, many of them may have had European ethnicity), East Asian (5%, although a further 14% described themselves as Asian Canadian and we suspect many of these were East Asian), and Indigenous (4%). In addition, 14% did not report their ethnicity. Measures Four Predictive Measures Were Collected at Time 1. Authentic Living and Authentically Resisting Influence (Wood et al., 2008). This model of authenticity includes three constructs: being true to oneself (“authentic living”), resisting external influences, and not feeling alienated from oneself, and has some evidence for validity (Wood et al., 2008). Authentic living (four items, for example, “I think it is better to be yourself than to be popular,” α = .69) adheres closest to the authenticity construct that our community coalition suggested for the study. The second construct involves resisting influence from others (four items, for example, “I usually do what other people tell me to do,” reverse scored, so higher scores indicated authenticity; α = .77). For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “not at all true” to 3 “very much true.” The self-alienation subscale was not included out of concern that the concepts would be too difficult for the youth of this age and could be conflated with dysphoria (e.g., “I feel alienated from myself”). Generalized Trust. Trust was assessed with the trust items from the Monitoring the Future Study and the General Social Survey, which ask the participants whether most people “can be trusted,” “would try to be helpful,” or “would try to be fair.” Each item had three response options that varied by question (e.g., for the first item: “Can’t be too careful,” “Don’t know,” or “Most people can be trusted”). The item scores are typically combined to create one score that functions in theoretically meaningful ways and thus has evidence for validity (Brehm & Rahn, 1997; Elgar & Aitken, 2011; Rahn & Transue, 1998; Stavrova & Ehlebracht, 2016; Twenge et al., 2014). The items have had limited internal consistency reliability (α = .45) but have nonetheless shown some similar and expected patterns of relations with other variables (Oishi et al., 2011). In this study, however, because of the limited internal consistency, the trust items were not combined but were instead used as separate variables, as has been previously done by Oishi et al. (2011). Trait Forgiveness Scale (Berry et al., 2005; Berry & Worthington Jr, 2001). The Trait Forgiveness scale has 10 items, including “I can usually forgive and for- 6 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) get an insult” and “I feel bitter about many of my relationships.” Cronbach’s alpha was .72 in this sample. For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “not at all true” to 3 “very much true.” Validity is supported by negative relations with vengeful rumination, and positive relations with third-party ratings of respondents and scenario-based measures of forgiveness (Berry et al., 2005; Berry & Worthington, 2001). Gratitude Questionnaire-8 (GQ-8). The GQ-8 is a youth version of the wellvalidated GQ-6 (McCullough et al., 2002) provided by an author of the GQ-6 (R. A. Emmons, personal communication, 2009). The GQ-6 has structural and convergent validity (Froh et al., 2011) with youth except for one item reflecting an extended time period (“Long amounts of time can go by . . .”). The GQ-8 replaces two items of this type with four new items (e.g., “When good things happen to me, I think of the people who helped me.”). In addition, the wording was slightly simplified by using “thankful” rather than “grateful.” Cronbach’s alpha was .77 in this sample. For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “not at all true” to 3 “very much true.” Five Outcome Measures Were Collected at Time 2. Self-Report of Offending (Huizinga et al., 1991). The self-report of offending included 21 items from an original 23-item scale assessing behaviors such as assault, theft, and fraud. Participants reported which of these they had committed since completing the first questionnaire (approximately 6 months earlier). The questionnaire asked, “Since our last survey, have you . . .” and listed each action (e.g., “Stolen something from a store (shoplifted)?”), and participants circled “No” or “Yes.” The scale has good psychometric properties (Knight et al., 2004) including associations with theoretically linked constructs, such as arrests, peer delinquency, and impulsivity. We removed two sex-related items due to the age of the participants. For each participant, we counted how many different types of offenses had been committed. Beliefs Supportive of Aggression. This is also called the Attitude Toward Violence scale (Houston Demonstration Project, 1993) as adapted previously (Bosworth & Espelage, 1995, as cited in Dahlberg et al., 2005). Six items assessed the belief that violence is justified in a variety of social situations, for example, “If I walk away from a fight, I’d be a coward (‘chicken’)”; α = .73 in this sample. Some validity support is provided by evidence that these items differentiate youth who bully or engage in dating violence (Bosworth et al., 1999; Smith-Darden et al., 2017). For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “strongly disagree” to 4 “strongly agree.” Tweed et al. 7 Little’s Pure Overt and Relational Aggression Scales (Little et al., 2003). The pure overt scale includes physical and other types of aggression (e.g., “I’m the type of person who hits, kicks, or punches others,” α = .88). The pure relational scale includes items such as “I’m the kind of person who tells my friends to stop liking someone” (α = .83). For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “not at all true” to 3 “very much true.” Some evidence for validity comes from correlations with self-rated and peer-rated hostility and peer-rated antisocial behavior (Little et al., 2003). Gang Attitudes Subscales (Winfree et al., 1994) as adapted by Esbensen (2003). We assessed attitudes toward gangs because gangs facilitate violence (Thornberry et al., 2002; Winfree et al., 1994) and because our community coalition was focused on gang issues. Four items assessed gang approval (a = .83 in our sample) by asking participants about their approval of gangrelated behavior (e.g., “doing whatever the gang leaders tell you to do”). For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “strongly disapprove” to 4 “strongly approve.” Higher scores on these items are associated with gang involvement (Esbensen, 2003; Winfree et al., 1994). Perceived gang rewards (α = .85 in this sample) items asked participants whether each of seven particular events would occur if they were involved in a gang (e.g., “I would get money”). Perceived gang consequences (α = .86) items asked whether bad consequences would occur (e.g., “I would lose my nongang friends”). The original, shorter, 10-item rewards and consequences questionnaire was developed by Winfree et al. (1994) [later expanded by Esbensen (2003)] and was correlated with gang involvement. Teacher Reported Reactive and Proactive Aggression (Dodge & Coie, 1987). Teachers provided ratings of reactive aggression (e.g., “When this teen has been teased or threatened, he or she gets angry easily and strikes back”) and proactive aggression (e.g., “This teen threatens or bullies others to get his or her own way”); α = .92 and .93, respectively, in this sample. Each subscale had three items, and possible teacher responses ranged from 1 “never true” to 5 “almost always true.” The sample size was smaller (n = 266) for this measure due to some teachers or parents choosing not to participate in this part of the study. Teacher ratings on these scales have been correlated with researcherobserved reactive and proactive aggression (Dodge & Coie, 1987). Three Indicators of At-Risk Status Were Gathered at Time 1. Impulsivity. Impulsivity was assessed using the seven highest loading items from the Eysenck and Eysenck (1978) measure of impulsiveness (e.g., 8 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) “I often get into a jam because I do things without thinking”). Some support for validity comes from the fact that scores on the overall scale are elevated among conduct-disordered youth (Daderman, 1999; White et al., 1994). The scale was shortened because adequate internal consistency was anticipated with fewer items (α = .83 in this sample) and because the classroom time available for the survey was limited. For each item, possible responses ranged from 0 “not at all true” to 3 “very much true.” Peer Delinquency. We assessed peer delinquency with eight items asking how many of one’s friends (none, few, some, or most) had engaged in particular acts (e.g., “Used a weapon or force to get money. . .,” “stole something worth more than $100”; α = .86 in this sample). For each item, possible responses included “none,” “few,” “some,” and “most..” The items were adapted from the Rochester Youth Development Study (Thornberry et al., 1994). The original items were associated with delinquency (Thornberry et al., 2002). One item had a weaker relationship with the other items and assessed a behavior not widely perceived as lawbreaking (“Skipped classes without an excuse”), so we replaced it with a gang-related item (“How many of your friends are in a gang?”); alpha rose from .75 to .82. School Failure. We assessed school failure with a single item asking, “How often do you get failing grades?” The response options were “never,” “sometimes,” or “a lot.” Procedures If a parent actively consented and the youth was in class on the day of the study, the youth was provided with an assent form and a chance to participate. Youth were provided with pizza and were entered into a raffle to win an electronic item. Questionnaires were read aloud in class, and youth recorded their answers on their own copy. When consent or assent was not provided, no demographic or other information was gathered. Approximately 6 months later, a second session was scheduled. Teachers were also provided with questionnaires for the youth if parents consented. Approximately, 9% (n = 34) of participants in the Wave 1 survey were not present at Wave 2. The research was approved by a university ethics board. Analyses First, to assess the predictive power of the indicators of optimal functioning, partial correlations were calculated between the predictors at Time 1 and the Tweed et al. 9 outcome variables at Time 2 (approximately 6 months later)—these partial correlations controlled for gender and age. Participants were included in any analysis for which they had sufficient data. Second, regression analyses were conducted to assess whether optimal functioning offered predictive power beyond that provided by the risk variables. The predictors were entered in this order: demographics (gender and age), risk factors (peer delinquency, school failure, and impulsivity), and indicators of optimal functioning. Because of the many similar relations shown in Table 1, and to reduce the number of analyses, subsequent analyses examined fewer outcomes. The regression outcomes included (a) selfreported offending; (b) a composite variable computed by taking the first unrotated principal component of beliefs supportive of aggression, Little’s Aggression scales, gang approval, and perceived gang rewards; and (c) teacher reports of proactive and reactive aggression. The self-reports of offenses were kept separate from other variables because some practitioners may have a particular interest in predicting this variable. The combined selfreport outcomes related to violence and aggression had similar results in Table 1, and all had loadings more than .70 on the first unrotated principal component of these variables (.77 for aggressive beliefs, .79 overt aggression, .71 reactive aggression, .73 positive gang attitudes, and .71 gang approval). Principal component analysis is primarily a data reduction method, so directly meets the goal of reducing the number of variables. The high loadings suggest that separate analyses for these would have been somewhat redundant. Third, moderation analyses were conducted to assess whether the genders differed in terms of relations between optimal functioning and outcomes, although we had no hypotheses regarding gender. Fourth, regression analyses were used to assess whether increased risk (indicated by school failure, delinquent peers, and impulsivity) weakened relations between optimal functioning and outcomes. We would have had 21 interaction terms if we included all seven predictors and the three risk factors. Combined with four outcomes, this would have produced 84 hypothesis tests. This could increase the likelihood of chance results. Therefore, for this analysis, we used only one optimal functioning variable (the first unrotated principal component of the optimal functioning indicators that emerged as predictors in the partial correlations) and one risk variable (the first unrotated principal component of school failure, delinquent peers, and impulsivity) and their interaction term. The variables within each component shared some variance as shown by the following: Each predictor (gratitude, forgiveness, authentic living, and the three trust questions) had a loading more than .4 on the predictor principal component. Each risk indicator (impulsivity, delinquent peers, and school failure) had a loading more than .55 on the 10 –.36*** .18 –.08 –.16** –.23*** –.41*** –.34*** –.17** .08 –.08 –.12* –.15** –.23*** –.26*** Violent beliefs –.10 –.15** –.19*** –.33*** –.27*** –.23*** .17** –.11* –.20*** –.25*** –.37*** –.33*** –.32*** .21 Overt Relational aggression aggression Note. Gender and age were controlled for in these partial correlations. *p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001. Time 1 Authenticity Living Resisting Positive regard Generalized trust   Trustworthy   Helpful   Fair   Forgiveness   Gratitude Offenses Time 2 –.12* –.12* –.24*** –.24*** –.26*** –.21*** .15** Gang reward –.04 –.02 .06 .07 .29*** .31*** –.12* Gang harm Gang approval –.06 .00 –.16** –.15*** –.24*** –.30*** .16** Gang attitudes Table 1. Partial Correlations Between Optimal Functioning (Time 1) and Outcomes (Time 2). –.13* –.04 –.08 –.16* –.17** –.20*** –.08 Reactive aggression –.04 –.01 –.09 –.06 –.06 –.20*** –.01 Proactive aggression Ratings by teacher Tweed et al. 11 risk component. This use of principal components has potential to increase statistical power by reducing error score variance and reduce the likelihood of erroneous results by reducing the number of analyses while still achieving the goal of assessing whether optimal functioning had less predictive power for youth with elevated risk. Results Table 1 shows the partial correlations between humanistic optimal functioning and outcomes. The results suggest that authentic living and positive regard (gratitude, forgiveness, and some elements of generalized trust) predict low levels of several violence-related variables 6 months later. The weakest findings were for the teacher reports of outcomes. The findings for one element of authenticity, resisting influence, were the opposite of expectations: resisting influence predicted beliefs supportive of violence and positive attitudes toward gangs. The regression results in Table 2 suggest that indicators of optimal functioning can sometimes provide incremental predictive validity beyond that accounted for by the risk factors included here. In particular, the indicators of optimal functioning offered incremental validity for predicting the composite of violence-related self-report variables and for predicting teacher reports of reactive aggression. The results for individual optimal functioning variables are shown in Table 3. Those are the same regressions as in Table 2, but Table 2 shows overall results for each block of variables rather than results for each optimal functioning variable within the optimal functioning block. Gender did not moderate the optimal functioning predictions except for teacher reports of proactive aggression, which was the one variable least predicted by optimal functioning. The gratitude interaction had a positive beta weight (β = .74, p = .004), meaning that the gratitude effect predicted more strongly for females. In contrast, authentic living had a negative beta weight (i.e., β = –.70; in the direction of authentic living predicting more strongly for males). In sum, gender interactions were the exception rather than the tendency. As shown in Table 4, the interaction between risk level (school failure, delinquent peers, and impulsivity) and optimal functioning was significant. In particular, optimal functioning predicted more strongly for youth at otherwise greater risk of perpetrating violence. For example, in predicting offending, the beta weight on optimal functioning is –.115 when risk is at the mean, but –.395 when risk is elevated 1 SD. This suggests that at-risk youth may show an especially strong negative relation between humanistic optimal functioning and offending. Similarly, in predicting the self-report of 12 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) Table 2. Time 1 Variable Blocks Predicting Time 2 Outcomes (Testing for Optimal Functioning Providing Prediction Beyond That Provided by Risk Factors). Time 2 Outcomes predicted Time 1 Predictor blocks ΔR2 F df p value Self-reported offenses Demographics Risk factors*** Optimal function .009 .166 .023 1.897 28.269 1.697 2/424 3/421 7/414 .151 <.001 .108 Violence self-report composite Demographics Risk factors*** Optimal function*** .011 .202 .120 2.466 38.466 11.358 2/453 3/450 7/443 .086 <.001 <.001 Teacher report of reactive aggression Demographics*** Risk factors* Optimal function* .035 .018 .035 7.589 2.647 2.269 2/421 3/418 7/411 .001 .049 .028 Teacher report of Demographics*** proactive aggression Risk factors Optimal function .034 .012 .027 7.366 1.792 1.709 2/421 3/418 7/411 .001 .148 .105 Note. Blocks of predictors were entered into the regressions in the following order: Demographics, risk factors, and then optimal functioning. *p ≤ .05. ***p ≤ .001. violence-related variables, the beta for optimal functioning is –.346 when risk is at the mean but becomes –.494 when risk is 1 SD above the mean. This suggests that at-risk youth may show an especially strong negative relation between humanistic optimal functioning and violence-related outcomes. A reviewer offered the insightful comment that authenticity may operate differently amid collectivism. As a first response, we examined the correlation between collectivism and the authenticity measures but found no statistically significant result. Testing interactions with collectivism seemed a reasonable next step, but we were hesitant to test many cultural interactions because this path could result in many hypothesis tests (and the related problems of increasing family-wise error). Thus, we focused on conducting four regressions testing the interaction between an indicator of collectivism and authenticity in predicting the outcomes. In particular, a dichotomous collectivism variable was created that identified individuals born in collectivistic countries. Significant interactions suggested that the authentic living variable may have less predictive power among the collectivistic participants for prediction of total offenses, the composite self-report violence variable, and for teacher reports of proactive aggression. The results suggest this is an avenue Tweed et al. 13 Table 3. Optimal Functioning Variable Results for Time 1 Predicting Time 2 Outcomes. Time 2 outcome Time 1 predictor β t p value Self-reported offenses Violence self-report composite score Block not significant Forgiveness** Gratitude** Trust: Trustworthy Trust: Helpful Trust: Fair* Authentic resisting* Authentic living*** –.134 –.122 –.036 .030 –.099 .089 –.179 –2.876 –2.643 –.885 .729 –2.386 2.142 –4.080 .004 .009 .376 .466 .017 .033 <.001 Teacher report of reactive aggression Forgiveness Gratitude Trust: Trustworthy Trust: Helpful Trust: Fair Authentic resisting* Authentic living* –.021 –.039 –.074 .034 –.012 –.104 –.109 –.368 –.690 –1.481 .677 –.241 –2.052 –2.038 .713 .490 .139 .498 .810 .041 .042 Teacher report of proactive aggression Block not significant Note. Table 2 shows results for blocks of variables. Table 3 relies on the same regressions, but shows results for individual variables in the optimal functioning block. No beta weight is shown for a term when the addition of the optimal functioning block of variables did not produce a significant change in R2. *p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001. worthy of further exploration, but we are hesitant to make strong conclusions, in part because this indicator of collectivism was confounded with other variables such as being a cultural minority and possibly other cultural variables such as vertical cultural orientation (Sivadas et al., 2008; Triandis, 1996). It is worth considering the possibility that in collectivistic cultures, affirming the value of authenticity may have a different meaning that lacks a protective element. We are hesitant, however, to make strong conclusions from these culture-related interactions although they do raise interesting possibilities. Discussion The results of these analyses suggest that humanistic optimal functioning has relevance to youth violence. Authentic living and positive regard for others (generalized trust, gratitude, and forgiveness) predicted low levels of 14 1/450 2/453 2/451 43.138 Interaction of Opt. Funct. and Risk*** .071 Predicted: Violence self-report composite score Demographics .011 2.466 Opt. Funct. and Risk*** .271 84.881 Variables .086 <.001 –.346 .234 –.148 –8.107 5.364 –3.584 –2.619 6.180 –6.568 t <.001 <.001 <.001 .009 <.001 <.001 p value Note. Beta values reflect the regression equation after the last statistically significant block of variables had been added. Thus, no beta weight is shown for a term when the addition of that block did not produce a significant change in R2. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001. Optimal. Funct.*** Risk*** Opt. × Risk*** –.115 .279 –.280 β Betas for individual variables .133 <.001*** Optimal. Funct.** Risk*** <.001*** Opt. × Risk*** p value Interaction of Opt. Funct. and Risk*** .020 12.843 1/450 <.001 Predicted: Teacher report of proactive aggression: Interaction not significant Predicted: Teacher report of reactive aggression: Interaction not significant 2/453 2/451 2.026 48.554 .009 .176 df Predicted: Self-reported offenses Demographics Opt. Funct. and Risk*** F ΔR2 Predicted Variables and Predictors Results for blocks of predictors Table 4. Regressions Assessing Interaction Between Optimal Functioning and Risk Level When Predicting Outcome. Tweed et al. 15 violence-related variables approximately 6 months later. The indicators of optimal functioning offered incremental predictive power beyond that provided by the risk factors. Interactions suggest that the predictive power of humanistic optimal functioning may be greatest among youth at heightened levels of risk. The results may seem self-evident to humanistic psychologists, but violence researchers may seldom draw from humanistic psychology for predictive variables (Coupland & Olver, 2020). Thus, there is value in providing empirical evidence for the relevance of humanistic psychology. Humanistic optimal functioning may deserve inclusion in assessments of violence potential and, if these effects are causal, within prevention and intervention efforts. Indicators of optimal functioning may be especially relevant for youth scoring high on risk indicators. Authentic living, one of the indicators of optimal functioning studied here, predicted low violence-related outcomes. Authenticity has long occupied a central role in humanistic psychology (Rogers, 1957; Tohme & Joseph, 2020). Authenticity is an interesting variable because it has been valued among philosophers (Sartre, 1975) and within popular media, including even children’s entertainment in the form of exhortations to follow one’s passions. Moreover, authenticity is associated with personal well-being (Sutton, 2020). It was surprising that the authenticity subscale of resisting influence predicted higher rates of violence-related outcomes in the cross-lag partialcorrelations. Perhaps the resisting influence measure, in this case, particularly captured readiness to reject messages from authority figures, something commonly seen in adolescents. This type of resistance among adolescents could possibly tend to promote or at least predict violence-related acts or dispositions. On the suggestion of a reviewer, we tested interactions between collectivism and authenticity. Authenticity may have a different meaning in a collectivistic context. The results suggested that the authentic living variable may have less predictive power among collectivistic youth. This finding may be worth exploring in future research. The measure of collectivism we had available was far from ideal, however, so although the finding is intriguing, we suggest treating this collectivism result as unclear evidence for now. Positive regard for others, another indicator of optimal functioning studied here, was assessed with indicators of generalized trust, forgiveness, and gratitude. Forgiveness requires letting go of resentment and having a more positive view of others who have brought harm. Gratitude requires a willingness to consider the positive benefit that others have brought to one’s life. These both require more positive regard rather than their opposite. 16 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) Generalized trust predicted lower levels of violence-related outcomes here, and it has broader relevance. Trust is also associated with psychological well-being (Oishi et al., 2011). When discussing generalized trust, it is important to represent the construct accurately. We used the word “trust” because that is the label used by other researchers (Mewes et al., 2021), but in laypersons’ usage, trust denotes making oneself vulnerable to harm from those on whom you rely. Youth, maybe especially youth who live in risky environments, need to be careful regarding in whom they place their trust. Shusako Endo (2008) wrote engagingly about the wonderful fool who trusted broadly, brought positive change to others, but suffered much harm. This study, however, and the prior studies, did not assess whether participants made themselves vulnerable to others but instead assessed the belief that most people are trustworthy. A person who scores high may still be careful to avoid risk. The term “faith in humanity” may communicate the idea better, although it encompasses more than generalized trust (Tweed et al., 2021). Concerningly, generalized trust has declined across recent decades in American samples (Mewes et al., 2021). Forgiveness and gratitude also predicted outcomes in this study. For forgiveness, one could speculate that the effect occurs because forgiveness displaces vengeful attitudes (Berry et al., 2005) that can motivate violence (Kennedy, 2011). For gratitude, some work suggests it contributes to prosocial intentions (Tsang, 2006) and reduces antisocial behavior more broadly (Bono et al., 2019). The results here are consistent with those findings. Tools assessing protective factors for violence can have great value (de Vogel et al., 2011). Trait forgiveness, faith in humanity (Tweed et al., 2021), authentic living, and gratitude do not tend to be included in these devices (e.g., Coupland & Olver, 2020). Perhaps that deserves reconsideration. Some limitations deserve mention. The relevance of a school sample to clinical populations could be debated. Nonetheless, the negative relation between indicators of optimal functioning and violence-related outcomes was strongest for youth at risk, suggesting relevance to more troubled samples. In addition, offense data and much of the violence data were selfreported. In addition, follow-up beyond 6 months would have value. Furthermore, the mechanism of the authentic living effect deserves further research. Authentic living may restrain violence. Alternatively, one could consider that society permits only some people to be authentic (those with conforming values) while pressuring nonconformers to hide their true self and beliefs. It could be that people who match societal norms are given permission to be themselves and the resulting positive social experience may restrain violence. Another limitation results from the fact that the generalized trust measure used here had problems of internal consistency, so the items Tweed et al. 17 were kept separate as has been done previously for the same reason (Oishi et al., 2011). The measure has a long history (Rosenberg, 1956), has evidence for convergent validity from expected correlations with other variables (Brehm & Rahn, 1997; Rahn & Transue, 1998; Stavrova & Ehlebracht, 2016; Twenge et al., 2014), and has generated large effect sizes (Elgar & Aitken, 2011). Perhaps it has worked sufficiently well in the past in spite of low internal consistency because the items tap distinct domains within generalized trust; however, other measures of similar constructs have been developed (Kaufman et al., 2019; Stavrova & Ehlebracht, 2016). Because of problems with the three-item trust measure, future studies may benefit from using one of the more recently developed measures. One caution deserves special mention. These results could easily be misinterpreted, most likely by people who are not humanistic theorists, to suggest that indicators of optimal functioning are largely fixed characteristics of individuals. To the extent that optimal functioning is unchangeable, then youth lacking these indicators could be seen by some as being doomed to a proclivity for violence. In contrast, evidence suggests that many widely known predictors of violence are dynamic (Douglas & Skeem, 2005). Thus, the potential for violence can change over time within individuals, possibly depending partly on their current environment. Thus, these indicators of optimal functioning could be changeable rather than fixed traits. Conclusion Several indicators of optimal humanistic functioning predicted lower scores on measures potentially related to violence. The indicators included authentic living and positive regard toward others (generalized trust, gratitude, and forgiveness). The optimal functioning measures sometimes provided incremental prediction beyond that offered by the risk factors included here. The predictive power of optimal functioning tended to be greatest among youth at heightened levels of risk (i.e., youth who were impulsive, had trouble in school, and had delinquent peers). The predictive power of optimal functioning indicators has implications for the assessment and possibly even reduction of risk for violence. To apply these results, assessment experts could more often include humanistic variables in their assessments. These analyses support that approach. Many youth, however, live in conditions that inhibit flourishing. To promote optimal functioning, community psychologists could seek to improve societal conditions, and therapeutic experts could provide interventions for individuals. Admittedly, however, the analyses here provide evidence of longitudinal predictive power and a possibility of causality but 18 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) cannot provide proof of the causal path toward violence reduction. Thus, directly testing the impact of humanistic interventions on violence will continue to have value, especially in light of the fact that even interventions that seem obviously valuable, sometimes fail to produce their expected effect (Gelfand et al., 2022) and in light of the potential power of efficacy data for persuading others. One significant concern exists. A focus on characteristics of persons potentially prone to violence could evoke a tendency to blame the individuals for their risk level or for not showing optimal functioning. In contrast, a humanistic perspective suggests humans are malleable and can grow in response to healthy environments. The humanistic psychology perspective suggests that the path forward involves not blaming and marginalizing but instead building conditions facilitating positive regard, empathy, and compassion toward those at increased risk of acting out in violence (Rogers, 1957). Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (Grant No. 833-2008-1028) supported this research. ORCID iD Roger G. Tweed https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4961-7944 References Adams, D. (1989). The Seville statement on violence and why it is important. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 29(3), 328–337. Atieno Fisher, J. (2003). Curtailing the use of restraint in psychiatric settings. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 43(2), 69–95. https://doi. org/10.1177/0022167802250578 Berry, J. W., Worthington, E. L., O’Connor, L. E., Parrott, L., & Wade, N. G. (2005). Forgivingness, vengeful rumination, and affective traits. Journal of Personality, 73(1), 183–226. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00308.x Berry, J. W., & Worthington, E. L Jr. (2001). Forgivingness, relationship quality, stress while imagining relationship events, and physical and mental health. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(4), 447–455. https://doi.org/10.1037/00220167.48.4.447 Tweed et al. 19 Bono, G., Froh, J. J., Disabato, D., Blalock, D., McKnight, P., & Bausert, S. (2019). Gratitude’s role in adolescent antisocial and prosocial behavior. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 14(2), 230–243. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1 402078 Bosworth, K., & Espelage, D. L. (1995). Teen conflict survey. Center for Adolescent Studies, Indiana University. Bosworth, K., Espelage, D. L., & Simon, T. R. (1999). Factors associated with bullying behavior in middle school students. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(3), 341–362. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431699019003003 Brehm, J., & Rahn, W. (1997). Individual-level evidence for the causes and consequences of social capital. American Journal of Political Science, 41(3), 999– 1023. https://doi.org/10.2307/2111684 Buber, M. (1957). Pointing the way: Collected essays (M. Friedman, Trans.). Harper & Brothers. Coupland, R. B. A., & Olver, M. E. (2020). Assessing protective factors in treated violent offenders. Psychological Assessment, 32(5), 493–508. https://doi. org/10.1037/pas0000807 Daderman, A. M. (1999). Differences between severely conduct-disordered juvenile males and normal juvenile males. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(5), 827–845. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00186-X Dahlberg, L. L., Toal, S. B., Swahm, M., & Behrens, C. B. (2005). Measuring violence-related attitudes, behaviors, and influence among youths: A compendium of assessment tools (2nd ed.). National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. de Vogel, V., de Vries Robbé, M., de Ruiter, C., & Bouman, Y. H. A. (2011). Assessing protective factors in forensic psychiatric practice: Introducing the SAPROF. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 10(3), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/14999013.2011.600230 Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158. Douglas, K. S., & Skeem, J. L. (2005). Violence risk assessment: Getting specific about being dynamic. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11(3), 347–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.3.347 Elgar, F. J., & Aitken, N. (2011). Income inequality, trust and homicide in 33 countries. The European Journal of Public Health, 21(2), 241–246. https://doi. org/10.1093/eurpub/ckq068 Endo, S. (2008). Wonderful fool (F. Mathy, Trans., 3rd ed.). Peter Owen Publishers. Esbensen, F.-A. (2003). Evaluation of the Gang Resistance Education and Training (GREAT) program in the United States, 1995–1999 (2nd ICPSR version). Interuniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research. Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Impulsiveness and venturesomeness. Psychological Reports, 43(3f), 1247–1255. https://doi.org/10.2466/ pr0.1978.43.3f.1247 Franco, Z., Hooyer, K., Ruffalo, L., & Frey-Ho Fung, R. A. (2020). Foreword to special issue on veterans health and well-being: Collaborative research 20 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) approaches. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 61, 287–312. https://doi. org/10.1177/0022167820919268 Froh, J. J., Fan, J., Emmons, R. A., Bono, G., Huebner, E. S., & Watkins, P. (2011). Measuring gratitude in youth. Psychological Assessment, 23(2), 311–324. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0021590 García-Vázquez, F. I., Valdés-Cuervo, A. A., & Parra-Pérez, L. G. (2020). The effects of forgiveness, gratitude, and self-control on reactive and proactive aggression in bullying. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(16), 5760. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165760 Gelfand, M., Li, R., Stamkou, E., Pieper, D., Denison, E., Fernandez, J., Choi, V., Chatman, J., Jackson, J., & Dimant, E. (2022). Persuading republicans and democrats to comply with mask wearing: An intervention tournament. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 101, 2–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jesp.2022.104299 Houston Demonstration Project. (1993). Peer leader survey. City of Houston Health and Human Services Department. Huizinga, D., Esbensen, F.-A., & Weiher, A. W. (1991). Are there multiple paths to delinquency? The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 82(1), 83–118. https://doi.org/10.2307/1143790 Kaufman, S. B., Yaden, D. B., Hyde, E., & Tsukayama, E. (2019). The light vs. dark triad of personality: Contrasting two very different profiles of human nature. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 467. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00467 Kennedy, D. M. (2011). Don’t shoot: One man, a street fellowship, and the end of violence in inner-city America. Bloomsbury. Knight, G. P., Little, M., Losoya, S. H., & Mulvey, E. P. (2004). The self-report of offending among serious juvenile offenders cross-gender, cross-ethnic/race measurement equivalence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 2(3), 273–295. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204004265878 Little, T., Henrich, C., Jones, S., & Hawley, P. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000128 Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. Harper & Row. McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J.-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 112–127. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112 Mewes, J., Fairbrother, M., Giordano, G. N., Wu, C., & Wilkes, R. (2021). Experiences matter: A longitudinal study of individual-level sources of declining social trust in the United States. Social Science Research, 95, Article 102537. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2021.102537 Morrill, Z. (2021). Introduction to the COVID-19 second special issue. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 61(2), 155–159. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0022167820966412 Muir, N. M., Viljoen, J. L., Jonnson, M. R., Cochrane, D. M., & Rogers, B. J. (2020). Predictive validity of the Structured Assessment of Violence Risk in Youth Tweed et al. 21 (SAVRY) with Indigenous and Caucasian female and male adolescents on probation. Psychological Assessment, 32(6), 594–607. https://doi.org/10.1037/ pas0000816 Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., & Diener, E. (2011). Income inequality and happiness. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1095–1100. https://doi.org/10.1177/095 6797611417262 Otto, R. K., & Douglas, K. S. (2009). Handbook of violence risk assessment. Taylor & Francis. Rahn, W. M., & Transue, J. E. (1998). Social trust and value change: The decline of social capital in American youth, 1976–1995. Political Psychology, 19(3), 545– 565. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00117 Rogers, C. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21(2), 95–103. https://doi. org/10.1037/h0045357 Rogers, C. (1972). Some social issues which concern me. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 12(2), 45–60. Rogers, C. (1980). A way of being. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Rogers, C. (2012). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of psychotherapy. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. http://www.myilibrary.com?id=521599 Rosenberg, M. (1956). Misanthropy and political ideology. American Sociological Review, 21(6), 690–695. https://doi.org/10.2307/2088419 Sartre, J. P. (1975). Existentialism is humanism. In W. Kaufmann (Ed.), Existentialism from Dostoyevsky to Sartre (pp. 345–368). Penguin Press. Sivadas, E., Bruvold, N. T., & Nelson, M. R. (2008). A reduced version of the horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism scale: A four-country assessment. Journal of Business Research, 61(3), 201–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .jbusres.2007.06.016 Smith-Darden, J. P., Kernsmith, P. D., Reidy, D. E., & Cortina, K. S. (2017). In search of modifiable risk and protective factors for teen dating violence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(2), 423–435. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12280 Sohmer, O. R. (2020). The experience of the authentic self: A cooperative inquiry. Journal of Humanistic Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi. org/10.1177/0022167820952339 Stavrova, O., & Ehlebracht, D. (2016). Cynical beliefs about human nature and income: Longitudinal and cross-cultural analyses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(1), 116–132. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000050 Sutton, A. (2020). Living the good life: A meta-analysis of authenticity, well-being and engagement. Personality and Individual Differences, 153, Article 109645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109645 Thornberry, T. P., Krohn, M. D., Lizotte, A. J., Smith, C. A., & Tobin, K. (2002). Gangs and delinquency in developmental perspective. Cambridge University Press. Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D., Farnworth, M., & Jang, S. J. (1994). Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology, 32(1), 47–83. https://doi .org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1994.tb01146.x 22 Journal of Humanistic Psychology 00(0) Tohme, O., & Joseph, S. (2020). Authenticity is correlated with mindfulness and emotional intelligence. Journal of Humanistic Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022167820940926 Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51(4), 407–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003066X.51.4.407 Tsang, J.-A. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: An experimental test of gratitude. Cognition and Emotion, 20(1), 138–148. https://doi .org/10.1080/02699930500172341 Tweed, R. G., Bhatt, G., Dooley, S., Spindler, A., Douglas, K. S., & Viljoen, J. L. (2011). Youth violence and positive psychology: Research potential through integration. Canadian Psychology/psychologie Canadienne, 52(2), 111–121. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0020695 Tweed, R. G., Mah, E. Y., & Conway, I. I. I. (2021). Bringing coherence to positive psychology: Faith in humanity. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 16(3), 298–309. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2020.1725605 Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Carter, N. T. (2014). Declines in trust in others and confidence in institutions among American adults and late adolescents, 1972–2012. Psychological Science, 25(10), 1914–1923. https://doi .org/10.1177/0956797614545133 White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., & StouthamerLoeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relationship to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(2), 192–205. https://doi .org/10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.192 Winfree, L. T., Bäckström, T. V., & Mays, G. L. (1994). Social learning theory, selfreported delinquency, and youth gangs: A new twist on a general theory of crime and delinquency. Youth & Society, 26(2), 147–177. https://doi.org/10.1177/004 4118X94026002001 Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic personality: A theoretical and empirical conceptualization and the development of the Authenticity Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(3), 385–399. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.385 Zych, I., Farrington, D. P., & Ttofi, M. M. (2019). Protective factors against bullying and cyberbullying: A systematic review of meta-analyses. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45, 4–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.06.008 Author Biographies Roger G. Tweed is a faculty member with the psychology departments at Kwantlen Polytechnic University in Surrey, British Columbia (BC), Canada and at Douglas College in New Westminster, BC, Canada. His studies currently focus on topics related to the nature of well-being and also on beliefs about positive and negative traits of other people. Tweed et al. 23 Gira Bhatt is a faculty member in the psychology department at Kwantlen Polytechnic University. She is the cochair of the Canadian Cross-Cultural Round Table on Security (Federal) and an executive member of the South Asian Community Coalition Against Youth Violence, BC. As the principal investigator of the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC)-funded CommunityUniversity Research Alliances (CURA) research project, she collaborated with 12 community partners to address the issue of youth violence and gang involvement. She received the 2018 Distinguished Contribution to Public or Community Service Award from the Canadian Psychological Association. Her primary focus is on translating research findings to community action plans and knowledge dissemination through popular media. Stephen Dooley became the second executive director of the Simon Fraser University (SFU) Surrey campus in November 2013. Prior to joining the team at SFU, he worked at Kwantlen Polytechnic University for more than 20 years, where he was the founder of the Centre For Interdisciplinary Research: Community Learning and Engagement (CIR:CLE). He has a strong commitment to the development of community–campus partnerships, based on the principles of equity, diversity, and inclusion. Jodi L. Viljoen was an associate professor of clinical-forensic psychology and the associate director of the Institute for the Reduction of Youth Violence. She published more than 50 articles and chapters, and made more than 150 presentations. She received the Saleem Shah Award for Early Career Contributions. Sadly, she passed away in 2022. Kevin S. Douglas is professor of clinical-forensic psychology, at SFU, in Burnaby, Canada. He is also a researcher at the Center for Research and Education in Forensic Psychiatry, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway, and senior research advisor, Center for Research and Education in Forensic Psychiatry, Oslo University Hospital, Norway. He has roughly 200 publications on topics related to violence, risk assessment, and psychopathy. Nathalie Gagnon is a faculty member in the criminology department at Kwantlen Polytechnic University and a Certified Organizational Coach. Her interests are currently centered on teamwork, systems thinking and learning organizations.